Skip to main content

A community-informed list of key speciation concepts for undergraduate education

Abstract

Background

Many topics in evolutionary biology have been the focus of little research about student thinking and learning. This lack of research limits the evidence base on which instructors can draw to inform their teaching. A key starting place for education research about evolutionary topics is determining what concepts are important for undergraduates to learn. This work develops a community-informed list of key concepts about speciation. Speciation is commonly taught in undergraduate biology education, yet has been the focus of almost no research on teaching and learning. We gathered input from over 110 evolution educators and speciation researchers to create a comprehensive list of speciation concepts for undergraduate education.

Results

The community-informed list includes 24 concept statements organized within 4 overarching concepts. At least 80% of experts rated these statements as scientifically accurate and clear. Over 90% of experts rated the statements as important or somewhat important for a graduating senior in biology to understand.

Conclusions

This list provides a foundation for both education researchers and evolution educators. Education researchers who investigate student thinking and who develop research-based measurement tools can use this list to determine key concepts on which to focus theirĀ future work. Educators can use this list to guide the development of learning objectives for speciation instruction. Future work should investigate what concepts are reasonable for an undergraduate to master in a 4-year degree.

Background

Currently there is little research about teaching and learning many topics in evolution. A systematic analysis of peer-reviewed literature related to undergraduate evolution education revealed many gaps in our existing knowledge base (Ziadie and Andrews 2018). Specifically, there has been little (or no) research about student thinking and learning of numerous evolutionary topics, including macroevolution, speciation, population genetics, quantitative genetics, life history evolution, and more. In contrast, research on how students think about and learn natural selection and tree-thinking has been much more common (Ziadie and Andrews 2018). This trend has continued despite repeated calls for additional education research related to non-adaptive evolutionary processes and macroevolution (e.g., Padian 2010; Novick et al. 2014; Price and Perez 2016). The outcome of this under-emphasis is a limited evidence base on which instructors can draw to inform their teaching.

In addition to the underrepresentation of particular evolutionary topics in education research, few researchers have aimed to establish standards for what college students should learn about evolution (Ziadie and Andrews 2018). Standards, which could include key concepts for students to learn, are an important starting place for education research. Research that aims to investigate student thinking and learning, develop research-based measurement tools, or develop and evaluate specific instructional strategies must focus on particular concepts or skills related to an evolutionary topic. For example, researchers aiming to investigate how college students reason about population genetics must choose particular population genetics concepts to study. These decisions would be facilitated if important evolutionary topics were broken down into key concepts that were agreed to be important by the evolution education community.

Two efforts have made some progress in this direction. The team behind the ā€œUnderstanding Evolutionā€ website created a framework of age-appropriate evolution concepts across grades K-16 (Understanding EvolutionĀ Conceptual Framework). This framework is aligned with both the Framework for K-12 Science Education (NRC 2012) and Next Generation Science Standards (NGSS Lead States 2013). It includes concepts for undergraduates, but these concepts are much less extensive than the evolution topics commonly taught in upper-division evolution courses (Ziadie and Andrews 2018). Therefore, this framework is best aligned with introductory biology instruction. Another research team expanded on the five core concepts in Vision and Change (AAAS 2011) by generating a framework of specific conceptual statements. This framework, called the BioCore Guide, incorporated feedback from more than 240 biologists and educators (Brownell et al. 2014). It describes evolution concepts in more detail than the Understanding Evolution framework, but still excludes many of the evolution topics taught in upper-division courses (Ziadie and Andrews 2018). Thus, the existing frameworks are insufficient to guide researchers who want to investigate student thinking and learning about specific evolutionary topics.

Our work aimed to establish a list of key concepts related to an important and overlooked evolutionary topic: speciation. Speciation was taught in over 95% of surveyed upper-division college evolution courses, and yet has been the focus of only two papers about assessment (e.g., Nadelson and Southerland 2010; Romine and Walter 2014) and a handful of non-empirical papers presenting instructional strategies (Ziadie and Andrews 2018). Not a single paper has investigated how undergraduates think about and learn speciation, nor are there studies determining appropriate speciation concepts for undergraduates to learn.

Thus, we aimed to develop a community-informed and comprehensive list of key speciation concepts for graduating college students. We drew on the expertise of speciation researchers and college faculty who teach speciation to build and refine a list of concepts. We present this as an aspirational list of concepts to be taught in an undergraduate curriculum focused on evolution.

Methods

Generating initial statements

We generated a preliminary list of speciation concept statements by consulting various undergraduate evolution, biology, and genetics textbooks and relevant peer-reviewed literature. We used these resources to identify how educators and researchers defined and explained speciation concepts, which concepts they emphasized, and how they organized concepts. We iteratively refined this list as a research team to create succinct statements that could then be evaluated by the community. Our preliminary concept list consisted of five main statements that had two to eight sub-statements each, for a total of 24 concept statements. We presented these concepts to community members via a series of online surveys. We used an iterative series of three surveys to gather feedback and revise concept statements. We both added and removed concepts based on expert feedback. We returned to the literature as needed to gain further insight.

Participant identification and recruitment

We recruited speciation experts to provide feedback on the list of speciation concepts. We recruited participants in three rounds and each sample evaluated a slightly different list of key concepts. We recruited participants in the same way for each of the three surveys. We identified experts in two different ways. We recruited some experts who had taught speciation in an upper-division undergraduate course. We expected these individuals to have valuable insight about what concepts are important and reasonable for undergraduates to learn about speciation. These experts had to have taught in evolution in an upper-division course in the last 3Ā years. We identified these experts by searching for courses focused on evolution within institutions of higher education that grant 4-year degrees. We identified these institutions from the Carnegie website (Carnegie Classification of Institutions of Higher Education). We filtered the list of institutions to only include institutions that offered 4-year degrees. We randomized this list, started at the top, and proceeded down the list. For each included institution, we used the institutionā€™s website to identify relevant courses and the instructors who had taught that course in recent years. We then found those instructorsā€™ email addresses from their institutionā€™s website.

We also recruited experts who were active speciation researchers. We expected these individuals to have valuable input about what concepts are most important to the field. These experts had published peer-reviewed research related to speciation within the last 3Ā years. We identified these experts via peer-reviewed journals that publish research related to evolutionary biology including The Journal of Evolutionary Biology, Evolution, Molecular Ecology, American Journal of Botany, New Phytologist, Systematic Entomology, Journal of Evolution, Ecology and Evolution, Molecular Biology and Evolution, Molecular phylogenetics and Evolution, and Current Zoology.

Though we recruited experts in two different ways, we expect many participants drew on expertise gained through both research and teaching. In total, we sent email recruitments to 706 potential expert participants in and outside of the United States. This included 211 faculty who had recently taught an upper-division course and 495 speciation researchers. Overall, 16% (nā€‰=ā€‰111) of those who received recruitment emails completed most or all of the survey about which they were contacted. Speciation researchers made up slightly more than half of the total expert participants (TableĀ 1). Of 111 participants, many (70%) had taught speciation in an upper-division course in the last 3Ā years and 60% had recently published peer-reviewed articles related to speciation (TableĀ 1). We conducted a total of three rounds of surveys and each expert participated in only one of these rounds. We opted for this approach to minimize the burden on any individual participant and to maximize the diversity of experts involved.

TableĀ 1 Number of participants, recruitment approach, and teaching, by survey

Data collection

We used three online surveys to gather expert feedback about the scientific accuracy, clarity, and importance of each statement of a speciation concept. The survey presented the concept list and asked experts to rate each concept as either ā€œAccurate and clearā€ or ā€œInaccurate and/or Unclear.ā€ If a participant rated a concept as ā€œInaccurate and/or unclear,ā€ the survey prompted them to provide feedback about how to edit the statement to be scientifically accurate and clear. The survey also asked experts to rate each concept as ā€œImportant,ā€ ā€œSomewhat important,ā€ or ā€œNot Importantā€ for a graduating senior in biology. Lastly, the survey presented the full list of concepts and asked experts if it was complete and, if not, what suggestions they had for additions. Each survey was identical except for the key concepts. See the full survey questions in the Additional file 1. We revised the list of key concepts and the articulation of each concept after Survey 1, and repeated the same process with a new and larger sample of experts in Survey 2. Experts responding to Survey 2 encountered only the revised key concepts, not the prior concepts. We made further revisions based on feedback collected in Survey 2 to create a list that a new sample of experts evaluated in Survey 3.

Data analysis

We calculated the percentage of experts from Survey 3 who rated each statement as ā€œAccurate and clear,ā€ and the percentage who rated each statement as ā€œImportantā€ or ā€œSomewhat important.ā€ Our cut-off for including a statement in our final community-informed key concept list was 80% of Survey 3 experts agreeing that the statement was ā€œAccurate and clear.ā€ We chose 80% as our criterion for consensus, rather than something higher, for two reasons. First, species definitions and delimitations continue to be debated within the scientific community (e.g., Wheeler and Nixon 1990; Mayden 1997; de Queiroz 2007), so we expected some disagreement among the experts we surveyed. Second, we aimed to articulate fine-grained key concepts, potentially making consensus more difficult to achieve than for more general statements. In contrast, researchers creating the BioCore Guide and determining key concepts in evolutionary medicine aimed to craft more general statements. BioCore creators used a 90% cut-off for agreement of importance and scientific accuracy (Brownell et al. 2014), and the evolutionary medicine team accepted 80% of participants strongly agreeing or somewhat agreeing with the importance of a statement as a mark of consensus (Grunspan et al. 2017). In the end, we did not need to have a separate inclusion criterion based on importance ratings because experts reported that most statements were important for students to learn. The only statement about which there was substantial disagreement regarding importance also garnered disagreement about accuracy, and thus was excluded from the final list.

Results

The community-informed list of key speciation concepts produced by this work includes 24 statements organized within four overarching concepts (TableĀ 2). Experts rated the full statements, and the research team generated the short codes for the sake of succinctly communicating data. More than 80% of experts responding to Survey 3 rated each of these statements as being ā€œAccurate and clear,ā€ with two exceptions that are described below. When asked to rate the importance of concepts for a graduating college senior to know about speciation, 91 to 100% of experts rated each statement as either ā€œimportantā€ or ā€œsomewhat importantā€ (TableĀ 3). Most statements were rarely rated as ā€œnot important,ā€ including 10Ā statements that never received this evaluation from experts.

TableĀ 2 Full key speciation concepts and short codes
TableĀ 3 Expert rating of key speciation concepts for accuracy and clarity, as well as importance

Two statements (SM4, SC3, TableĀ 2) did not achieve the threshold of being considered accurate and clear by 80% of surveyed experts. One statement nearly reached this threshold (SM4, 78%, TableĀ 3) and we opted to retain it in the final list so that researchers and educators could make their own judgement. Feedback from experts allowed us to propose a revision to the other statement, SC3, which is about the phylogenetic species concept. This statement required revision because only 63% of experts rated the version of the statement in Survey 3 as accurate and clear. Another version of this statement was rated as accurate and clear by 77% of experts in Survey 2. Expert feedback indicated the need to synthesize the statement versions from Surveys 2 and 3. Specifically, experts argued for the inclusion of diagnosability, which is the ability to recognize populations because they possess a unique combination of character states. They also valued the inclusion of monophyly and the existence of unique shared derived characters (apomorphies) in a statement of the phylogenetic species concept. Thus, as shown in TableĀ 2, we propose a community-informed version of the phylogenetic species concept that includes both of these criteria. It is important to note that this version of the statement has not been evaluated by experts, but was created with expert feedback in mind.

Experts evaluated most of the final statements in Survey 3, but two were evaluated in Survey 2 (SC1, SM1, TableĀ 2). Revisions to these statements between Survey 2 and Survey 3 decreased the proportion of experts who rated them as accurate and clear, so we retained the versions of the statements from Survey 2, in which 84% and 83% of experts rated as accurate and clear, respectively. Additionally, one statement about the ecological species concept was omitted from our final list entirely because repeated revisions based on feedback did not result in consensus among experts about the accuracy or the importance of the statement.

Discussion

This work lays a foundation on which evolution education researchers can build. We have identified a comprehensive list of speciation concepts that educators and researchers agree are important for biology graduates to understand. Researchers who study student cognition and learning can use this list to select specific speciation concepts to explore further. Research on student cognition and learning can then lay the groundwork for developing research-based measurements of undergraduatesā€™ thinking about speciation. Educators can use this list as they determine what concepts are key for their students to learn.

We offer one caution about the comprehensiveness of this list. This list represents the concepts that evolution experts see as important, but it does not consider what is feasible for undergraduates to master during a 4-year degree. It is possible that the level of the detail included in this list, if combined with this level of detail about other topics in biology, would be an unreasonable expectation for most undergraduates. It might be more reasonable to think that students develop this level of conceptual knowledge in the first years of graduate school. Thus, we present this list as tentative, and we encourage researchers to investigate how undergraduates learn these concepts, so that a final list of key speciation concepts is built not just from the expert perspective, but also from a student-centered perspective of learning.

A key concept list is only a starting place for speciation instruction. A critical next step will be aligning learning objectives with these key concepts. These concepts were not written nor evaluated to be statements presented to students. Furthermore, they do not illuminate what students should be able to do if they understand a key concept. For example, key concept SC2 describes the biological species concept. Most basically, instructors might aim for students to achieve this learning objective: ā€œDefine the biological species concept.ā€ We would advocate that upper-division courses aim for more advanced learning objectives, such as, ā€œUse the biological species concept to evaluate data and determine if a population should be managed as one or more species.ā€ It will be important for the evolution education community to discuss and aim for consensus about how key concepts should be translated into concrete learning objectives.

We encourage researchers and educators to look closely at the data about importance (TableĀ 3), which may suggest ways to limit this list to only the most critical concepts. If we used a cut-off of 80% of experts rating a statement as ā€œImportant,ā€ the key concept list would be reduced from 24 to 8 concepts. That said, experts were not asked to rank statements so these values do not represent relative importance. Rather, experts rated the importance of each statement independently, so statements with values greater than 80% were rated as important by more than 80% of experts. Some experts may have rated every key concept as important whereas others may have rated only some as important.

We anticipate that some readers will find fault with this list of speciation concepts. This potential for disagreement was reflected in our survey results. It was not uncommon for experts to provide feedback in direct contradiction to feedback from other experts. We offer this list of key speciation as a starting place that garnered approval from most, but not often all, surveyed experts. See Additional fiel 2 for the written feedback provided by surveyed experts and anonymous reviewers regarding each key concept in the final list.

We also discuss a few areas of disagreement among experts that influenced the final list of key speciation concepts. First, this list of speciation concepts does not explicitly name the various geographical contexts in which speciation may occur (i.e., allopatry, sympatry, parapatry). Early versions of concept statements emphasized how barriers to gene flow may arise within different geographical contexts and defined allopatric, sympatric, and parapatric modes of speciation. However, experts disagreed about the utility of discussing modes of speciation by focusing on geography, and instead advocated for greater focus on processes of speciation. Modern speciation research often focuses on different evolutionary processes that drive genetic divergence (e.g., Funk 1998; Mani and Clarke 1990; Ramsey and Schemske 1998; Schluter 2009; Maan and Seehausen 2010). This is partly because speciation research has progressed to the extent that the different evolutionary processes driving speciation can be tested directly (Butlin et al. 2008). SM2 (TableĀ 2) recognizes that geographical isolation often contributes to speciation, and SM4 (TableĀ 2) recognizes that natural selection can lead to speciation with or without geographical isolation. We do not dispute that geographic separation is important to speciation; nor did most of the surveyed experts. However, definitions of allopatric, sympatric, and parapatric speciation are not part of the key concept list.

We also want to highlight that though we have included species concepts in the community-informed list, this is an area of historical and ongoing debate. Different biologists have advocated the use of various species concepts, and the inconsistencies between these concepts can lead to differing conclusions concerning the number of species that exist. For example, Mayden (1997) identified 24 species concepts that he recognized as distinct, many of which are at least somewhat incompatible. Evolutionary geneticists and systematists both concern themselves with species and speciation, but focus primarily on understanding the processes of speciation and the taxonomy of diversity, respectively. These differing scholarly goals may lead them to advocate different concepts of a species. Thus, it is not surprising that this sample of expertsā€”selected based on their expertise in speciation writ largeā€”had diverse ideas about the utility of different species concepts and their relative importance.

Two experts in our study suggested the inclusion of the Unified Species Concept (de Queiroz 2007). De Queiroz (2007) argues that all species concepts include the common idea that species are a separately evolving metapopulation lineage, and that this should be the only necessary property of species. Other properties, such as reproductive isolation and monophyly, would then be considered different lines of evidence for assessing lineage separation that can be acquired by lineages as they diverge (de Queiroz 2007). Ultimately, we did not include a key concept statement explicitly about the Unified Species Concept. Instead, statement SC1 describes species as ā€œa population or group of populations that experiences evolutionary processes independently from other populationsā€ (TableĀ 2). We recommend that upper-division speciation instructors consider how the ideas of de Queiroz (2007) fit into their courses and consider the 2007 paper as a possible introduction for students to the diversity of and disagreement regarding species concepts.

De Querioz is not alone in suggesting that different species concepts address fundamentally different entities (e.g., Ereshefsky 1992, Baum and Shaw 1995). For example, Harrison (1998) proposes that the phylogenetic species concept and biological species concept can be viewed as different stages in the speciation process and that the order of these stages may vary based on the geography of speciation. These discussions may have a place in upper-division courses.

There are other resources specifically written for educators and students that may usefully supplement traditional textbook readings in introducing students to key speciation concepts. Understanding Evolution is an education website that provides both readings and lesson materials related to evolutionary biology (https://evolution.berkeley.edu/evolibrary/home.php). The resources therein are particularly useful for lower-division undergraduate courses. Other resources target more advanced students. Scitable by Nature Education (https://www.nature.com/scitable) includes articles written for students on topics in evolutionary genetics. For example, Johnson (2008) addresses hybrid incompatibility and speciation, Stevison (2008) discusses hybridization and gene flow, and Hey (2009) discusses species concepts. Similarly, eLS Citable reviews in the life sciences (http://www.els.net/WileyCDA/) includes overviews of topics in evolution written at appropriate levels for college instructors and students. These vary in specificity from an overview of species concepts by Ghiselin et al. (2010) to an evolutionary history of polar and brown bears by Hailer and Welch (2016). eTS resources require access to Wiley Library Online.

Conclusions

This paper presents a community-informed list of key speciation concepts. We hope it will prompt additional research and discussion about the understandings we aim to cultivate in students earning a degree in biology. We also hope it offers a jumping off point for research about how undergraduates think about these important speciation concepts. Researchers would benefit from a research-based assessment of student thinking about speciation that is grounded in the concepts valued by the community. Educators can use this list, or parts of it, as a conceptual framework for planning learning objectives for students.

Availability of data and materials

All data analyzed in this manuscript is available upon request from Tessa Andrews (tandrews@uga.edu)

Abbreviations

SM2:

speciation mechanism 2

SM4:

speciation mechanism 4

SC3:

speciation concept 3

SC1:

speciation concept 1

SM1:

speciation mechanism 1

References

  • American Association for the Advancement of Science. Vision and change: a call to action (Final report). Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science; 2011.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Baum DA, Shaw KL. Genealogical perspectives on the species problem. In: Hoch PC, Stephenson AG, editors. Experimental and molecular approaches to plant biosystematics, vol. 53. St. Louis: Missouri Botanical Garden; 1995. p. 123ā€“4.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Brownell SE, Freeman S, Wenderoth MP, Crowe AJ. BioCore guide: a tool for interpreting the core concepts of vision and change for biology majors. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2014;13(2):200ā€“11.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Butlin RK, Galindo J, Grahame JW. Sympatric, parapatric or allopatric: the most important way to classify speciation? Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2008;363:2997ā€“3007.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Carnegie Classification of Institutions of Higher Education. http://carnegieclassifications.iu.edu/. Accessed 2018.

  • De Queiroz K. Species concepts and species delimitation. Syst Bio. 2007;56(6):879ā€“86.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Ereshefsky M. Eliminative pluralism. Philos Sci. 1992;59:671ā€“90.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Funk DJ. Isolating a role for natural selection in speciation: host adaptation and sexual isolation in Neochlamisus bebbianae leaf beetles. Evolution. 1998;52:1744ā€“59.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Ghiselin, Michael T (2010) Species Concepts. In: eLS. Wiley, Chichester. http://www.els.net; https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470015902.a0001744.pub2.

  • Grunspan DZ, Nesse RM, Barnes ME, Brownell SE. Core principles of evolutionary medicine: a Delphi study. Evol Med Public Health. 2017;2018(1):13ā€“23.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Hailer F, Welch AJ (2016) Evolutionary history of polar and brown bears. In: eLS. Wiley, Chichester. http://www.els.net; https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470015902.a0026303.

  • Harrison RG. Linking evolutionary pattern and process. In: Howard DJ, Berlocher SH, editors. Endless forms: species and speciation. New York NY: Oxford University Press; 1998. p. 19ā€“31.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Hey J. Why should we care about species? Nat Educ. 2009;2:2.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Johnson N. Hybrid incompatibility and speciation. Nat Educ. 2008;1:20.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Maan ME, Seehausen O. Mechanisms of species divergence through visual adaptation and sexual selection: perspectives from a cichlid model system. Curr Zool. 2010;56:285ā€“99.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Mani GS, Clarke BC. Mutation orderā€”a major stochastic process in evolution. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1990;240:29ā€“37.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Mayden RL. A hierarchy of species concepts: the denouement in the saga of the species problem. In: Claridge MF, Dawah HA, Wilson MR, editors. Species: the units of biodiversity. London: Chapman & Hall; 1997. p. 381ā€“424.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Nadelson LS, Southerland SA. Development and preliminary evaluation of the measure of understanding of macroevolution: introducing the MUM. J Exp Educ. 2010;78(2):151ā€“90.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • National Research Council. A Framework for K-12 science Education: practices, crosscutting concepts, and core ideas. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2012. https://doi.org/10.17226/13165.

    BookĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • NGSS Lead States. Next generation science standards: for states, by states. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2013.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Novick LR, Schreiber EG, Catley KM. Deconstructing evolution education: the relationship between micro-and macroevolution. J Res Sci Teach. 2014;51(6):759ā€“88.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Padian K. How to win the evolution war: teach macroevolution! Evo Educ Outreach. 2010;3(2):206ā€“14.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Price RM, Perez KE. Beyond the adaptationist legacy: updating our teaching to include a diversity of evolutionary mechanisms. Am Biol Teach. 2016;78(2):101ā€“8.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Ramsey J, Schemske DW. Pathways, mechanisms and rates of polyploid formation in flowering plants. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 1998;29:467ā€“501.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Romine WL, Walter EM. Assessing the efficacy of the measure of understanding of macroevolution as a valid tool for undergraduate non-science majors. Int J Sci Educ. 2014;36(17):2872ā€“91.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Schluter D. Evidence for ecological speciation and its alternative. Science. 2009;323:737ā€“41.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Stevison L. Hybridization and gene flow. Nat Educ. 2008;1:111.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Understanding evolution conceptual framework. https://evolution.berkeley.edu/evolibrary/teach/framework.php. Understanding Evolution Website. University of California Museum of Paleontology. Accessed 13 Feb 2019.

  • Wheeler QD, Nixon KC. Another way of looking at the species problem: a reply to de Queiroz and Donoghue. Cladistics. 1990;6(1):77ā€“81.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Ziadie MA, Andrews TC. Moving evolution education forward: a systematic analysis of literature to identify gaps in collective knowledge for teaching. CBE Life Sci Educ. 2018;17(1):ar11. https://doi.org/10.1187/cbe.17-08-0190.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Thank you to the survey respondents who generously gave their time to provide their expertise about speciation concepts. Thanks also to Michelle Ziadie, for her early help and inspiration for this project. We thank two anonymous reviewers for feedback that improved the manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by the University of Georgia.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

DD helped conceptualize the study, collected and interpreted data, and drafted the manuscript. NE helped conceptualize the study, collected and interpreted data, and drafted the manuscript. TCA conceptualized the study, interpreted the data, and drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tessa C. Andrews.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Institutional Review Board of the University of Georgia determined this work (#00003496) was exempt.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional files

Additional file 1.

Survey used to gather expert feedback on speciation concepts.

Additional file 2.

Written comments provided by surveyed experts and anonymous reviewers regarding each key concept in final list.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dial, D., Emetu, N. & Andrews, T.C. A community-informed list of key speciation concepts for undergraduate education. Evo Edu Outreach 12, 14 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12052-019-0105-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12052-019-0105-2

Keywords