Skip to main content
  • RESEARCH ARTICLE
  • Open access
  • Published:

Visualizing macroevolutionary timescales: students’ comprehension of different temporal representations in an animation

Abstract

Background

Macroevolutionary time is a difficult idea to grasp and is considered to be a threshold concept in teaching and learning evolution. One way of addressing this subject is to use animations that represent evolutionary time. The aim of this descriptive and exploratory study was to investigate how various representations of time in an animation affect the way undergraduate students comprehend different temporal aspects of hominin evolution. Two factors, namely differences in timelines (the number of timelines with different scales) and the mode of the default animated time rate (either constant throughout the animation or decreasing as the animation progressed) were combined to give four different time representations. The temporal aspects were investigated using undergraduate students’ ability to find events at specific times, to comprehend relative order, to comprehend concurrent events, to estimate the duration of time intervals and their ability to compare the lengths of time intervals.

Results

The results revealed that “finding events at specific times” near to the end of the animation (closer to present time), where the sequence of events appeared very quickly, was more difficult for groups working with animations with only one timeline. We also found that the ability to comprehend concurrent events can be impaired if several timelines are displayed and the animation speed is relatively high. The ability to estimate the duration of a time interval was more difficult for groups working with animations with only one timeline, especially at the end of the animation where the sequence of events occurred quickly. Making correct comparisons of time intervals was relatively independent of which animation was used with one notable exception: groups working with an animation featuring several timelines and a decreasing default animated time rate performed worst at comparing events with intervals that spanned parts of the timeline with different scales.

Conclusions

Our results indicate that the choice of animation should depend on the teaching intention. However, a visualization with several timelines, and an animated time which slowed down toward present time, generated the best results for the majority of items tested. Temporal scale shift may interfere with the perception of time in cases where durations are compared.

Background

Challenges associated with teaching and learning evolution

The theory of evolution is considered to be the most important theory in biology, since it underpins all biological disciplines from ecology to molecular biology and biochemistry (Dobzhansky 1973). It explains how all life forms are related, how different organisms derive from a common ancestor, and how species have diversified. It has a bearing on issues from bacterial resistance to antibiotics as well as adaptations to climate change. Hence, evolution is clearly of critical importance for biology education and for understanding many aspects of fundamental science and the diverse challenges facing individuals, communities, and global ecosystems.

However, teaching and learning about evolution is challenging for different reasons. A number of teaching strategies and tools have been developed over the past 30 years (Andersson and Wallin 2006; McVaugh et al. 2011; Nehm and Reilly 2007; Nelson 2008; Sinatra et al. 2008) which have contributed to meaningful strategies for teaching evolution. Compilations of pedagogical and philosophical issues related to teaching and learning evolution have been published (Smith 2010a, b). Nevertheless, various obstacles regarding teaching evolution need to be further addressed by researchers and teachers. In fact, robust misconceptions relating to evolution are frequent among learners (Smith 2010a), and previous research has confirmed that there are a variety of problems facing teachers as well as for example curators at museums in communicating evolutionary content (Harcourt-Smith 2012). Hence, it is crucial to find the main difficulties and to identify new approaches to overcome them.

Some of the difficulties in understanding evolution stem from problems in comprehending underlying, more general, concepts. In recent literature on evolution teaching and learning, a new class of concepts, “threshold concepts”, have emerged (Meyer and Land 2003). They are defined as follows: “A threshold concept can be considered as akin to a portal, opening up a new and previously inaccessible way of thinking about something. It represents a transformed way of understanding, or interpreting, or viewing something without which the learner cannot progress.” (Meyer and Land 2003: 1). Several threshold concepts in association with evolution have been proposed among those is “temporal scale” (Ross et al. 2010), which in biology can span billions of years to instantaneous time. Strategies for how to incorporate threshold concepts, such as time, in teaching evolution is further discussed by Tibell and Harms (2017).

In this exploratory and descriptive study, we focus on one of the threshold concepts, time, and on how the design of visual representations in an animation affects students’ comprehension of evolutionary deep time. The study seeks to contribute to the discussion on how visualizations can be designed to facilitate teaching and learning about issues where time scales are important to understand.

“Time” in teaching and learning about evolution

Evolution is a complex process depending on of several factors, often involving very large and very small quantities such as large populations sizes, low mutation frequencies and enormous numbers of generations. In other words, understanding evolution requires the learner to be able to integrate phenomena across diverse temporal and spatial scales. Deep time is a fundamental element of this complexity. The definition of evolution according to Henderson’s dictionary of biology (Lawrence 2013: 218) includes “the development of new types of living organisms from pre-existing types by the accumulation of genetic differences over long periods of time.” This definition emphasizes the involvement of a vast number of generations in the development of new species, and for most multicellular organisms this means thousands to millions of years—deep time. Furthermore, comprehension of large numbers associated with deep time have proved to be challenging for many students (Cheek 2012). Integrating these scales in evolution lies completely beyond the human range of experience, and poses major educational challenges.

Representation of deep time

During the 17th and 18th centuries, the notion of the earth being very old emerged. The geologist James Hutton (1726–1797) coined the term “deep time”, referring to geological time, that is, time frames covering multiples of millions of years—an idea further developed by Charles Lyell (Lyell and Deshayes 1830). Deep time has subsequently been introduced into evolution theory. Several studies have emphasized the importance of learners’ understanding of deep time in evolutionary and geological education (Catley and Novick 2009; Dodick 2007; Trend 2001). However, timescales covering deep time are different from those we experience in our daily lives, hence they present challenges in understanding evolutionary processes (Catley and Novick 2009).

As deep time is an abstract domain, it is usually shaped by metaphorical mappings from more concrete domains, primarily space (Boroditsky 2000; Lakoff and Johnson 1999). This means that thinking about time usually occurs in terms of spatial-relational information. For example, elements and their relationships are represented as locations, and durations are represented as distances on a timeline (x-axes) (Boyd-Davis 2012; Lakoff and Johnson 1999). Provided with an animated cursor indicating time-passage, the animated rate of time can also be temporally changed (“compressed” or “expanded”) in relation to real time (i.e. that 1000 years for example can correspond to the distance the cursor has moved during 1 s). Thus, the timeline and the time-rate metaphors can be combined, by letting a cursor marking the time at the timeline at constant or varying rates (see “Representations of time in evolution” below).

In what is referred to as the “time orientation metaphor” (Lakoff and Johnson 1999), the observer is situated at the present, facing the future with the past behind. Since a common conceptual metaphor for time passing is motion in space, the time orientation metaphor can be combined with what Lakoff and Johnson (1999) term “the moving observer metaphor”. In this metaphor the observer is moving towards the future leaving the past behind with time conceptualized as a series of locations (Lakoff and Johnson 1999). Timelines with an animated cursor marking the present resonate with this combination of the time orientation metaphor and the moving observer metaphor. One way of making the abstract concept of time more concrete is to map it visually onto space in the form of timelines. This has been done over the last 250 years (Boyd-Davis 2012). The mapping of time to space also appears frequently in language and may even constitute a foundation to our reasoning and thinking about time (Boroditsky 2000).

Understanding deep time

The concept of deep time comprises several components and requires the ability to locate and order events in history over a large linear scale, as well as the ability to estimate a duration and to compare it to other events or processes (Cheek 2013a; Hidalgo et al. 2004). Furthermore, the ability to compare how long ago two events occurred from a fixed reference point is of importance (Hidalgo et al. 2004). An aspect of time that is less well represented in previous research is a learner’s ability to comprehend the concurrency of events. In addition, according to Cheek (2010, 2012), the ability to correctly conceptualize events in deep time depends on a solid conception of large numbers and the proportion between them (e.g. the difference between one million and one billion). Since most students do not have much experience of large numbers this can be challenging to relate to. Furthermore, subject-specific knowledge is essential to provide suitable reference points (Cheek 2013b; Clary et al. 2009).

Digital representations, time and learning

Developments in educational technology have become relevant for teaching and learning (Luckin et al. 2012; Moreno and Mayer 2007) and enable new ways of representing time (Kraak et al. 1997; Peterson 1999). Monmonier (1990) presented a conceptual framework for the graphical representation of geographic time series to guide designers. He distinguished between two strategies available in digital environments: an interactive system allowing the user to manipulate time series easily and animation graphics, where the sequence is ordered in time, turning the visualization into a model with spatial scales with different time rates.

Research in GIS (Geographical Information Systems) is extensive and might be used in reference to spatio-temporal animations. It has shown how different representations of time affect users. In a study carried out by Edsall et al. (1997), and later confirmed by Midtbø et al. (2007), three different representations of time were investigated: a timeline, a time wheel and a text-based legend. No significant differences in the performance among groups working with the different representations could be shown. Vít and Bláha (2012) studied time represented as a temporal axis, with time units of consistent length and a movable slide bar illustrating the rate of time, proved to be most user-friendly when compared to the representation with units of varying length and a movable slide bar moving at a constant speed.

Cognitive load and visualization of time

A well-documented aspect of multimedia learning is the magnitude of cognitive load associated with the visualization. A large body of basic research has been devoted to this particular area and general frameworks have been developed (Baddeley 2003; Moreno and Mayer 2007; Paas et al. 2003; Sweller 1988). These are based on assumptions about human knowledge such as the limited processing capacity (Baddeley 1992) and the dual channel assumption (Sweller 2005). The importance of choosing well designed animations in appropriate learning situations has also been stressed by Tversky et al. (2002). Of special interest for this study is the cognitive load attributed to the mental effort required to be spent on the manner in which information is presented (Paas et al. 2003). This type of load is largely determined by how instructional materials are designed.

Since more is usually known about the recent past than the distant past, there is a tendency for a natural overload of information as the timeline reaches the present day (Boyd-Davis 2013). This also applies to knowledge about evolutionary history since more information is available from recent times (e.g. fossils and organic remains are more abundant) compared to the material from more ancient times which have largely been decomposed. This type of “design problem” in visualizations of timelines with uneven distribution of events has also been addressed by Vít and Bláha (2012). From a design point of view, optimizing the narrative structure has meant making compromises between the need for “higher resolution”, in terms of a slow animated rate of time and detailed timelines during periods with a large number of events, and the need for a faster animated rate of time during periods with fewer events.

Representations of time in evolution

Visual representations have the potential to transform science education in making abstract and non-perceptual content concrete. In fact, Kozma et al. (2000) have shown that scientific progress is tightly connected to the ability to model abstract and complex content. Multimedia and dynamic visual representations, such as videos and animations, are approaches for connecting time frames and illustrating dynamic processes. In the context of evolution, animations and simulations could be useful to depict non-perceptual, complex and dynamic processes such as speciation and changes in abundance and distribution of species over a long time.

Given this important role of visual representations, Bohlin et al. (2017) have previously surveyed the presence of threshold concepts in 60 online dynamic visualizations. This study revealed a clear lack of certain thresholds such as small spatial and temporal scales. In a follow up study (Tibell et al. 2016), the presence of time and visual temporal scale indicators in 127 dynamic visual representations were analyzed. The most frequent category of threshold concepts was time, which was found in about 26% of the studied dynamic visual representations, and approximately as often orally narrated as visually shown. The processes were indicated at individual, generation and species level, and were visually conveyed through timers, geological strata in combination with timelines, calendars where dates and years past very quickly, or an episode of “fast forward”.

The timeline, as a device for organizing coherent sequences of events, has been the focus of many studies (Boyd-Davis 2012; Foreman et al. 2008; Korallo et al. 2012), which have included the construction of student generated timelines to investigate understanding of deep time (e.g. Cheek 2012; Clary et al. 2009; Libarkin et al. 2007; Truscott et al. 2006). The understanding of differences in temporal scale between events is poor but comprehension of relative order is better (Libarkin et al. 2007).

Literature suggests that how well a particular representation of time can support a learner in understanding temporal aspects of a sequence depends on many factors such as the learner’s existing knowledge and capabilities, as well as the design, purpose and context of the animation (Ainsworth 2008a). In this study, we have focused on design issues. From a didactic point of view, it is important that the design of any animation serves the intended educational purpose. However, there are many different aspects to consider, some of which compete and are even contradictory. For example, in spatio-temporal animations, there may be a need for a narrative flow, that is, a relatively even frequency of occurring events, coupled with the need to display prolonged time intervals with few events (Boyd-Davis 2013; Vít and Bláha 2012). The problem of uneven distribution of events requires consideration as to whether it is important that users should perceive the presented information intuitively or be able to process the information further through other processes such as making calculations. Therefore, an awareness of how different temporal representations serve different educational purposes is important for designers of applications, as well as for teachers choosing among the multitude of available animations.

One of the most common representations of deep time is a physical, linear and one-dimensional timeline placed in the corridor of a school (Truscott et al. 2006). Since it is known that students tend to overestimate very short durations and underestimate very long durations (Lee et al. 2011), this kind of linear representation can facilitate understanding of deep time. However, Lee et al. (2011) caution the use of visualizations that alter the representation of time rate for the same reason.

Other ways to communicate temporal information include the use of multiple representations, but the way time is displayed is crucial to consider (Ainsworth and VanLabeke 2004). General principles for how multiple external representations can serve several functions have been discussed by Ainsworth (1999). However, there is also considerable evidence that learners often fail to take advantage of multiple representations, and that they generally need to be used in a supportive context (Ainsworth 2008b).

A different way of representing time is through the use of evolutionary trees. The main goal in this regard is to illustrate relationships between species, but attempts to depict time are sometimes included. In most cases, however, time is not illustrated using a common time representation but rather through a convenient sliding scale (Horn et al. 2016).

A summary of some relevant factors that might hinder or improve the understanding of time scales is presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Important factors that might hinder or improve the understanding of time scales

Purpose of the study

There are few studies that have investigated how deep time and temporal scales affect teaching and learning evolution, and almost none on how visualizations can support learning in this regard. Since time can be illustrated in many different ways (Ainsworth and VanLabeke 2004) an important aspect to consider is the relationship between how dynamic representations affect student’s perception and comprehension of various temporal aspects.

In this exploratory and descriptive study, the investigated aspects are deduced from prior research in different areas, which include:

  • The ability to find events at specific times (Hidalgo et al. 2004).

  • The ability to comprehend the relative order of events (Catley and Novick 2009; Libarkin et al. 2007; Trend 1998, 2000, 2001).

  • The ability to estimate the duration of a time interval (Cheek 2013b; Dodick and Orion 2003).

  • The ability to compare different temporal durations (Cheek 2013a; Hidalgo et al. 2004).

  • The ability to comprehend the concurrency of events (Mayer and Moreno 2003).

We have constructed one animation with four versions of a time representation. Each of the time representations have been analyzed regarding these temporal aspects.

Methods

Setting and sample

This study involved 144 university students enrolled in a teacher education program while attending a biology course at a Swedish University. To obtain sufficient numbers of participants and to be able to develop the design of the study, we collected data in four rounds over 15 months (Table 2). The average age of the students varied between 22.6 and 24.5 years in the different rounds of data collection. The gender distribution was biased towards females (78%) which also reflects the current gender balance in recruitment for this type of education. We obtained consent to publish the anonymized results from all the participants.

Table 2 Participants in the different rounds of data collection

According to a questionnaire distributed before the study, the participants had a similar educational background and interests. Since hominin evolution had not been taught in any of the courses the students were tested concerning their pre-knowledge of hominin evolution. Furthermore, we used items to test students’ arithmetic skills, ability to interpret graphical representations of timelines, and to compare the size of graphical objects (see Appendix, questions 9–13). Since the test items (see Table 4 below) include mathematical skills the results did not indicate any notable difference between the groups.

The animations

In this paper, we will use the term “animation” to define a form of dynamic representation that displays processes that change over time. This conforms to the definition of animation used by Ainsworth (2008a).

Since it was relevant to the participants, and it was considered as a highly motivating topic for students in general, we chose the evolutionary history of hominins as an example of macroevolution (Bajd 2012; Besterman and Baggott La Velle 2007). The timescale involved in the visualization covers about 7 million years (Wood 2010), which is a relatively short time in evolutionary terms, but far beyond human experience.

We constructed four versions of a three-and-a-half minute long spatio-temporal animation specifically for this research project to investigate how different representations of time would affect learning. It was inspired by similar visualizations used in museums (e.g. Naturhistoriska riksmuseet in Stockholm, Sweden) and available on the internet.

The animations were designed as a map upon which animated areas were superimposed (Fig. 1). Each area represented the appearance, dispersal and disappearance of a chosen sample of different species, or groups of species in hominin evolution. The scenario was based on a selected sample of fossil findings covering hominin history (Wood 2010). All versions of the animation shared these features, the only difference being how time was represented. The visual representation of time was located below the map at the bottom of the animation and consisted of two components: one or several timelines with different scales, and an animated cursor moving horizontally along the timeline, indicating current time and rate of time in the animation. The participants were able to start, stop and replay the animation.

Fig. 1
figure 1

A visualization with the animated “current time cursor” positioned to the far right (i.e. close to present time). The timeline, visible in this image (timeline 1) span 7 million years. The areas superimposed on the maps represent the distribution of different hominin species, or groups of species, at any specific time

The animations used in the study can be accessed at http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:liu:diva-153235.

Different versions of the time representation

Much more is known about recent evolutionary history compared to more ancient times, just because the more ancient material has largely been destroyed. Therefore, the risk of information overload as the timeline reaches present day is difficult to avoid unless the default animated rate of time slows down, in which case perception of time might be affected. To investigate this issue, we constructed four variants of the same animation differing only in how time was represented.

We compared the effect of a time rate which varied versus a constant time rate. Furthermore, we compared the effect of a single scale versus emerging magnified scales by bootstrapping, that is, the method to use the full extent of one scale and link it to a fraction of another scale (Lee et al. 2011). These two factors, the time rate and the number of timelines with different scales were combined to yield four versions of time representations (Table 3). In the following text we use the expression rate of time to denote the default animated rate of time as it is represented in the temporal legend, unless otherwise stated.

Table 3 Combinations of animated rate of time and number of timelines with different scales (subsumed as the different temporal legends)

In animation A only one timeline was used (timeline 1), and the rate of time (indicated by an animated cursor) was the same throughout the animation, and was equivalent to 33,333 years per second (Fig. 2). However, timeline 1 was divided into three parts, the first covering 7 million years ago (7 Ma) to 2.3 Ma, the second covering the period between 2.3 and 0.23 Ma, and the last covering 0.23 Ma—present day. The indicator of time rate and current time was an animated cursor moving along the timeline.

Fig. 2
figure 2

The timeline (timeline 1) used in animations A and B. The cursor indicating time in the animation is visible in this figure to the far right

Animation B comprised, as A, of only one timeline (timeline 1, see Fig. 2) but the rate of time changed twice as the animation reached the second and the third parts of the timeline. The rate of time was 49,500 years per second during the first part of the timeline (blue) which covered the period 7–2.3 Ma ago, 25,000 years per second during the second part (brown) which covered 2.3–0.23 Ma ago and 7200 years per second during the most recent and final part (red) covering 0.23 Ma–present day.

In animation C, the rate of time was the same throughout the animation (as in A), but the uppermost timeline (timeline 1) was sequentially complemented with two timelines of increasing magnification scales (timelines 2 and 3, see Fig. 3) as the animation reached the second and third parts of timeline 1. This design is an example of shifting scales by bootstrapping (Lee et al. 2011).

Fig. 3
figure 3

The three timelines used in animations C and D. The uppermost is timeline 1, which looks the same in all of the animations. The two underlying timelines (2 and 3), shown below, represent fractions of timeline 1 but expressed in successively larger scales. Timelines 2 and 3 emerged as the current time cursor approached 2.3 and 0.23 Ma respectively before present day

In animation D, the timeline was constructed as in C, but the rate of time changed twice (as in animation B) as the animation reached the second and third parts of timeline 1 as in animation B, each speed reduction coincides with the appearance of timelines 2 and 3 below timeline 1, respectively.

Test items

We developed ten items to address the different aspects of time formulated in the research questions (Table 4). To elucidate differences caused by the different animations we grouped the items in pairs which corresponded to the five research questions. One of the items in each pair was expected to be in principle less independent of which animation that was used (i.e. earlier in the animation). The other item was expected to be dependent on which animation that was used, since it targeted events during the last 230,000 years where the scales and rate of time differed in the four animations.

Table 4 Test items used in the study

Study design

The study was designed to investigate how students perceived the depicted process while viewing one version of the animation (version A, B, C or D, see Table 3). The students worked individually, and were presented with two equally-sized windows shown concurrently on a computer screen. One displayed a specific version of the animation and the other displayed the items. An online survey tool (Limesurvey GmbH/LimeSurvey: An Open Source survey tool/LimeSurvey GmbH, Hamburg, Germany. URL http://www.limesurvey.org) was used to construct the questionnaire and collect data. The students worked with the items simultaneously as they viewed the animations since we were interested in the process of perceiving and comprehending the information while it was presented.

Data analysis

The students’ responses to the items were categorized as either “correct” or “incorrect”. The criteria used to categorize the answers concerning finding events at specific times were as follows: maximum 10% deviation from the correct answer were considered as correct, and deviations exceeding 10% deviation from the correct answer were considered to be “incorrect”. Since items addressing estimates of duration required estimates of two points in time, a maximum of in total 20% deviation from the correct answer was judged as “correct” and values exceeding 20% deviation from the correct answer were considered to be “incorrect”, and scored 0 points. The answer to item 3 “What different species of Hominins existed between the penultimate and the last ice age?” had a unique format which was categorized as correct if two or more species were named. To normalize the results, they are expressed as percent of correct answers for each group.

Results

Results concerning the participants’ ability to answer items related to the animations are presented in Table 5. The results are presented as average values for each group and item and the corresponding percent of correct answers. In the following, each research question is addressed separately.

Table 5 Results from item 1–10

Students’ ability to find events at specific times

Finding events at specific times located in the first part of timeline 1 (Fig. 2) was examined with the first item (“When did the group Australopithecus species appear?”). The different groups had similar success in answering this item. The fraction of students who answered correctly varied between 73 and 81% in the different groups.

The second item (“When did the last ice age begin?”) was apparently more difficult to answer. It addressed a point in time at the end of the animation, in the third part of the timeline, where all three timelines with increasing scales were visible in animations C and D (Fig. 3). The groups using animations with only one timeline (animations A and B, see Fig. 2) demonstrated difficulties. None of the participants could answer this item correctly while using the animation with a single timeline and a constant time rate (animation A), and 9% when the time rate was reduced (animation B). Groups using animations with three timelines of increasing scales (groups C and D) seem to succeeded better, 52% correct answers, and 54% respectively.

Students’ ability to comprehend the relative order of events

Two items addressed the ability to comprehend the relative order of two events. The first item asked “Did Homo habilis exist before the appearance of Homo erectus?” (item 5) and the second asked “Did Homo erectus exist before the appearance of Australopithecus?” (item 6). A high percentage in all groups (85–100%) answered these items correctly. Apparently these items were not difficult for the students, independent of whether the times to be located were situated early or late in the time frame and independent of which animation was used.

Students’ ability to comprehend concurrent events

In item 3 concerning concurrent events, the participants were asked to state the number of different species of Hominins that existed between the penultimate and the last ice ages which occurs very late in the animation. The groups using the different animations A, B and D performed relatively well and delivered between 68 and 85% answers.

Item 4 asked “Did Homo heidelbergensis exist at the same time as Homo sapiens?” This item concerns periods relatively near the end of the animation, where timeline 1 is supplemented with two additional timelines in animations C and D (Fig. 3). This item was related with 62–71% of correct answers in group A, B and D. For both item 3 and 4, group C performed the worst (50% and 48% respectively).

Students’ ability to comprehend durations of time intervals

Item 7 concerned an interval during the first part of the animation: “How long did the group with Sahelanthropus, Orrorin and Ardipithecus exist?” Regardless of which animation that was used, it should be equally difficult to answer this item correctly since all of the animations started in a similar way, with one timeline, few events and a similar rate of time. For some reason animations A and D appear to help the students to produce better answers (86% and 82%) than animation B and C (57–58%).

Item 8 was related to an interval occurring during the last part of the animation: “How long was the penultimate ice age?”. This item appeared to challenging for all students to solve. None of the students using animations A and B could answer this item correctly. Students using animations C and D (which featured three timelines during the last part of the animations) performed better but still only with 24% and 36% correct answers respectively.

Students’ ability to compare durations of time intervals

In item 9, the respondents were asked to “Compare the time Homo neanderthalensis existed with the time Homo habilis existed”. The existence of these species covered different parts of the animation where the rate of time changed in animations B and D, making the comparison harder for participants using these animations. Information concerning the length of time H. habilis and H. neanderthalensis existed could only be acquired from timeline 1 in animations A and B, whereas the additional timelines in animations C and D also provided the same information on scales with higher resolution. The percentages of correct answers for animations A and B (featuring a single timeline, see Fig. 2) are 36% and 38%, respectively. The students using animation C performed best with 52% correct answers and students using animation D performed worst, with 28% correct answers.

In item 10, the respondents were asked “How long did the group of genus Sahelanthropus, Orrorin and Ardipithecus exist compared to Homo sapiens?”. The group that viewed animation A performed worst and delivered 43% correct answers, whereas groups B and C produced similar results, namely 53% and 57%, respectively. The best results were obtained from group D, which had 74% correct answers.

Discussion

Results from research on how well animations aid users’ understanding of dynamic phenomena in general are ambiguous. Some indicate positive effects (Kaiser et al. 1992) and some negative (Schnotz et al. 1999; Tversky et al. 2002). However, studies of how diverse designs of time in animations help students to grasp different aspects of time (including deep-time) in evolution are scarce. In this descriptive and exploratory study we have investigated how various representations of time in an animation affect students’ experiences of different temporal aspects in an evolutionary scenario depicting hominin evolution. However, our results are to be taken as tentative, since they are not based on experimental data, and need to be validated experimentally.

We have chosen to represent time in the form of animated timelines. However, there are choices to be made concerning the design of such animated temporal representations. When representing evolutionary time, a timeline with linear scale has limitations. Even the relatively short evolutionary time of hominins (about 7 million years) is difficult to represent in this manner (Dunsworth 2011). Of course, this difficulty is even more pronounced when the entire evolutionary history of about 3.5 billion years is considered.

We discuss the results commencing with the cases where the design of time appeared to have had the least impact on students’ comprehension of evolutionary deep time, and closes with the instances where the design was observed to have a large effect.

The comprehension of relative order of events

The results indicate that there is no clear difference in comprehending the relative order of events between the different animations. The findings of this study support previous research on how temporal succession in deep time is experienced (e.g. Catley and Novick 2009; Libarkin et al. 2007; Trend 1998, 2000, 2001). Addressing simple sequencing was relatively easy for the participants, independent of which of the animations they used. This agrees with the observation that the learner does not necessarily have to use information from the timeline since the relative order of species can be learnt from the animated map directly.

The ability to comprehend concurrent events

It was also possible to comprehend concurrent events by watching the animated map without using the timeline. Several quickly occurring events during the last part of the animation made it harder to comprehend concurrent events for the groups using visualizations with a constant rate of time. The group working with animation C, which had three timelines and a constant rate of time, performed worst in both items whereas the groups working with animations featuring a slower pace at the end (animations B and D) performed the best. The most probable reason for this result is that several simultaneously and fast-occurring events cause a higher cognitive load (Kalyuga 2007). Furthermore, the animated timeline itself might also generate too much user attention. Thus, distracting viewers from relevant information displayed on the animated map (Peterson 1999).

The ability to find events at specific times and to estimate the duration of a time interval

Concerning the identification of events at specific times (items 1 and 2) and estimation of time duration (items 7 and 8), no obvious difference could be observed between groups using any of the four animations in the items related to the first part of the timeline (items 1 and 7). The reason is most probably that the rates of time in this part were relatively similar and the period contained fewer events. However, the two visualizations featuring only one timeline (animations A and B) generated poorer results for finding times and estimating durations during the third part, at the end of the animation (items 2 and 8). The spatial scale was a decisive factor in these items and restricted the ability to correctly determine time. In fact, it was very difficult for the students to read the times involving the last 230,000 years on the scale used in animations A and B. The relatively low number of completely correct answers also for animations C and D might be a consequence of a high cognitive load due to the emerging timelines and the eventful time frame (Paas et al. 2003).

The ability to compare different temporal durations

The ability to make comparisons between two durations (items 9 and 10) is the most complex factor investigated in this work. This task places great cognitive demands on the participant. First, it requires the students to be able to identify two intervals using both the information presented in the animated maps as well as the information expressed in the timeline. Second, students have to calculate the ratio of the intervals. Thus, two different abilities are required to solve items of this kind. The second ability is independent of the first, but both abilities are based on mathematical skills. However, since no difference in mathematical skills was measured between the participating groups in a test before the experiment, other factors seem to be dominant, primarily the fact of which animation was used.

Some of the features of the time representation in animation D, which possibly provide advantages in other aspects, might increase the cognitive load due to the built-in shifts in spatial and temporal scales, i.e. several timelines and varying rates of time. For example, in item 10, the required time intervals were located at opposite ends—in the first and third part of the animation, none of the intervals transgressed shifts in scales along the timeline. This might explain why the group using animation D succeeded best in answering item 10 but worst in item 9, in which changes in temporal and spatial scale complicated the comparison of durations.

Representation of time

When we consider the results with in respect to the presence of one or several timelines and whether the rate of time was constant or decreasing in the animation, it is possible to draw some tentative conclusions. The two animations featuring a single timeline (animations A and B, see Fig. 2) produced similar results. The shared characteristic of having only one timeline seems to be a more decisive factor rather than the difference in how the rate of time was expressed. The most salient results concern finding events at a specific times (item 2) and estimating the duration of a time interval (item 8) during the last part of the animation. Animations A and B gave rise to poorer results compared to the other animations. The reason for this is likely due to the small spatial scale during the event-filled final part of the animation, which made it harder to access detailed information from the timeline. In items involving comparisons of time intervals (items 9 and 10), the difference in results between the animations was less pronounced.

The animations featuring additional timelines as the animation progressed to the second and third part (i.e. animations C and D, see Fig. 3) offered the users increased spatial scales during the successively more event-filled periods at the end. Both these animations yielded better results than those with a single timeline for the items where events during the third and last part were involved—with one notable exception (item 9). Since animation C represented a constant rate of time, several rapid transient events occurred at the end, during the third part. Despite this, the group using this animation performed well in all of the items related to items 2, 8, 9 and 10. Overall, the animation that produced the best results was animation D, with the exception of item 9 in which two intervals crossing scale shifts were compared. In this case, the shift in scales might have introduced an additional cognitive load in addition to the perceived inconsistent temporal information in the two different dynamic components (i.e. the map and the timelines). The change of time rate is only apparent in the temporal legend and not explicitly shown on the animated map. Lee et al. (2011) caution the use of visualizations that alter the speed of changes. This study partly supports this claim but only when comparisons of durations which cross scale shifts are involved.

Limitations of the study

We want to stress that our results are tentative since they come from a descriptive and explorative study. Many of the results are similar to corresponding results in neighboring domains, which strengthen their validity. Other results have to be validated experimentally. However, even though this is a descriptive and exploratory study, might have benefitted from a larger sample of participants and a more balanced gender distribution.

Variations concerning which groups were involved in the different rounds could possibly contribute to differences in the results. In a potential next experimental setting of the study we need to address these shortcomings. Further, we could have used more test items to ensure reliability. However, it took about an hour for the students to answer the 10 items, and a longer form had probably jeopardize the quality of the answers due to exhaustion among the participants.

Implications for education and outreach

From the discussion above, it follows that there are several factors that need to be considered when using animations to communicate evolutionary scenarios involving deep time. A tradeoff has to be found between factors that might seem contradictory. For example, manipulation of the time rate can impair the ability to compare durations for example if the duration coincides with changes in time rate. The need to represent time in a simple and comprehensible way at the same time as handling problems with a very uneven distribution of events which might both cause cognitive overload and impair the narrative structure (Lee et al. 2011; Vít and Bláha 2012).

Since different ways to represent time have corresponding advantages and disadvantages, a way to circumvent potential conflicts between various factors would be to design a representation that offers different degrees of interactivity. In other words, with the potential to interact directly with the timeline to enlarge selected parts of it spatially and to control the animation speed easily (Kalyuga 2007; Moreno and Mayer 2007). Such interactivity could give the user control and a tactile, as well as visual, connection to the temporal information, making the experience more embodied (Horn et al. 2016; Lakoff and Johnson 1999; Peterson 1999). Future research is needed to investigate this broader and, in more detail. How should representation of macroevolutionary scenarios be designed to enhance the comprehension of time differences involving billions of years and temporal scale shifts? Our results suggest that teachers should be aware of the deficits in visualizations of time/temporal aspects of evolution. But most of all, the present results might inform guidelines for future improvements of educational visualizations of time in evolution

Conclusions

Through an analysis of four representations of time in a visualization of hominin evolution, we argue that informed and conscious choices of animations with appropriate depictions of time can facilitate the teaching and learning of subject matter regarding evolutionary scenarios involving deep time. In contrast to earlier research (Midtbø et al. 2007), this study indicates that different representations affect learners’ abilities to perceive and comprehend at least some temporal aspects. In line with results from geographic information systems studies, time units of consistent length were proven to be most helpful (Vít and Bláha 2012) in giving students a sense of the relative time spans of evolution, which is one of the most difficult aspects to master in understanding evolution. However, the intensity of events progressively increasing at the end of the animation poses another challenge when time units of consistent length are used. If specific times are of importance, it is crucial to be able to read the time, and the time scale should be adapted to that demand. Consequently, different ways of presenting the sequence of events is dependent on the specific educational purpose.

References

  • Ainsworth S. The functions of multiple representations. Comput Educ. 1999;33(2–3):131–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth S. How do animations influence learning? In: Robinson D, Schraw G, editors. Current perspectives on cognition, learning, and instruction: recent innovations in educational technology that facilitate student learning. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing; 2008a. p. 37–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth S. The educational value of multiple-representations when learning complex scientific concepts. In: Vis. Theory Pract. Sci. Educ. Dordrecht: Springer; 2008b. p. 191–208. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-5267-5_9. Accessed 18 Jan 2016.

  • Ainsworth S, VanLabeke N. Multiple forms of dynamic representation. Learn Instr. 2004;14(3):241–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersson B, Wallin A. On developing content-oriented theories taking biological evolution as an example. Int J Sci Educ. 2006;28(6):673–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley A. Working memory. Science. 1992;255:556–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley A. Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4(10):829–39.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bajd B. Human evolution and education in Slovene schools. Evol Educ Outreach. 2012;5(3):405–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besterman H, Baggott La Velle L. Using human evolution to teach evolutionary theory. J Biol Educ. 2007;41(2):76–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohlin G, Göransson A, Höst GE, Tibell LAE. A conceptual characterization of online videos explaining natural selection. Sci Educ. 2017;26(7–9):975–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boroditsky L. Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition. 2000;75(1):1–28.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd-Davis S. History on the line: time as dimension. Des Issues. 2012;28(4):4–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd-Davis. Inventing the timeline: a history of visual history. London; 2013. https://vimeo.com/58980227. Accessed 24 Jan 2018.

  • Catley KM, Novick LR. Digging deep: exploring college students’ knowledge of macroevolutionary time. J Res Sci Teach. 2009;46(3):311–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheek KA. Factors underlying students’ conceptions of deep time: an exploratory study. Durham University; 2010. http://etheses.dur.ac.uk/277/. Accessed 8 Oct 2015.

  • Cheek KA. Students’understanding of large numbers as a key factor in their understanding of geologic time. Int J Sci Math Educ. 2012;10(5):1047–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheek KA. Exploring the relationship between students’ understanding of conventional time and deep (geologic) time. Int J Sci Educ. 2013a;35(11):1925–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheek KA. How geoscience novices reason about temporal duration: the role of spatial thinking and large numbers. J Geosci Educ. 2013b;61(3):334–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clary RM, Brzuszek RF, Wandersee JH. Students’ geocognition of deep time, conceptualized in an informal educational setting. J Geosci Educ. 2009;57(4):275–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dobzhansky T. Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. Am Biol Teach. 1973;35(3):125–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodick J. Understanding evolutionary change within the framework of geological time. McGill J Educ. 2007;42(2):245–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodick J, Orion N. Cognitive factors affecting student understanding of geologic time. J Res Sci Teach. 2003;40(4):415–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunsworth HM. Deep time in perspective: an animated fossil hominin timeline. PaleoAnthropology. 2011;13:17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edsall R, Kraak M-J, MacEachren AM, Peuquet DJ. Assessing the effectiveness of temporal legends in environmental visualization. Cincinnati; 1997. p. 677–85.

  • Foreman N, Boyd-Davis S, Moar M, Korallo L, Chappell E. Can virtual environments enhance the learning of historical chronology? Instr Sci. 2008;36(2):155–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harcourt-Smith WEH. Up from the ape: the spitzer hall of human origins at the american museum of natural history. Evol Educ Outreach. 2012;5(1):4–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidalgo AJ, Fernando IESS, Otero ICEJ. Research report: an analysis of the understanding of geological time by students at secondary and post-secondary level. Int J Sci Educ. 2004;26(7):845–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn MS, Phillips BC, Evans EM, Block F, Diamond J, Shen C. Visualizing biological data in museums: visitor learning with an interactive tree of life exhibit: visualizing biological data in museums. J Res Sci Teach. 2016;53(6):895–918.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser MK, Proffitt DR, Whelan SM, Hecht H. Influence of animation on dynamical judgments. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1992;18(3):669.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga S. Enhancing instructional efficiency of interactive e-learning environments: a cognitive load perspective. Educ Psychol Rev. 2007;19(3):387–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Korallo L, Foreman N, Boyd-Davis S, Moar M, Coulson M. Can multiple “spatial” virtual timelines convey the relatedness of chronological knowledge across parallel domains? Comput Educ. 2012;58(2):856–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozma R, Chin E, Russell J, Marx N. The roles of representations and tools in the chemistry laboratory and their implications for chemistry learning. J Learn Sci. 2000;9(2):105–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kraak M-J, Edsall R, MacEachren AM. Cartographic animation and legends for temporal maps: exploration and or interaction. In: Proc. 18th Int. Cartogr. Conf. 1997. p. 253–261. http://www.geovista.psu.edu/publications/MacEachren/Kraak_etal_97.PDF. Accessed 17 Dec 2015.

  • Lakoff G, Johnson M. Philosophy in the flesh: the embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. New York: Basic Books; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawrence E. Henderson’s dictionary of biology. New York: Benjamin Cummings; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee H-S, Liu OL, Price CA, Kendall ALM. College students’ temporal-magnitude recognition ability associated with durations of scientific changes. J Res Sci Teach. 2011;48(3):317–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libarkin JC, Kurdziel JP, Anderson SW. College student conceptions of geological time and the disconnect between ordering and scale. J Geosci Educ. 2007;55(5):413–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luckin R, Bligh B, Manches A, Ainsworth S, Crook C, Noss R. Decoding learning. NESTA; 2012.

  • Lyell C, Deshayes GP. Principles of geology; being an attempt to explain the former changes of the earth’s surface, by reference to causes now in operation. London: J. Murray; 1830. https://www.biodiversitylibrary.org/item/107923. Accessed 28 Jan 2018

  • Mayer RE, Moreno R. Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educ Psychol. 2003;38(1):43–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • McVaugh NK, Birchfield J, Lucero MM, Petrosino AJ. Evolution education: seeing the forest for the trees and focusing our efforts on the teaching of evolution. Evol Educ Outreach. 2011;4(2):286–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer JHF, Land R. Threshold concepts and troublesome knowledge: linkages to ways of thinking and practising within the disciplines. University of Edinburgh UK; 2003. https://www.dkit.ie/ga/system/files/Threshold_Concepts__and_Troublesome_Knowledge_by_Professor_Ray_Land.pdf. Accessed 25 Nov 2014.

  • Midtbø T, Clarke KC, Fabrikant S. Human interaction with animated maps: the portrayal of the passage of time. In: 11th Scand. Res. Conf. Geogr. Inf. Sci. 2007. p. 45–60. http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Keith_Clarke2/publication/228897062_Human_interaction_with_animated_maps_The_portrayal_of_the_passage_of_time/links/0912f51356472b0d32000000.pdf. Accessed 8 Jan 2016.

  • Monmonier M. Strategies for the visualization of geographic time-series data. Cartogr Int J Geogr Inf Geovisualization. 1990;27(1):30–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreno R, Mayer R. Interactive multimodal learning environments: special issue on interactive learning environments: contemporary issues and trends. Educ Psychol Rev. 2007;19(3):309–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nehm RH, Reilly L. Biology majors’ knowledge and misconceptions of natural selection. Bioscience. 2007;57(3):263–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson CE. Teaching evolution (and all of biology) more effectively: strategies for engagement, critical reasoning, and confronting misconceptions. Integr Comp Biol. 2008;48(2):213–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paas F, Renkl A, Sweller J. Cognitive load theory and instructional design: recent developments. Educ Psychol. 2003;38(1):1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson MP. Active legends for interactive cartographic animation. Int J Geogr Inf Sci. 1999;13(4):375–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross PM, Taylor CE, Hughes C, Whitaker N, Lutze-Mann L, Kofod M, et al. Threshold concepts in learning biology and evolution. Biol Int. 2010;47:47–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnotz W, Böckheler J, Grzondziel H. Individual and co-operative learning with interactive animated pictures. Eur J Psychol Educ. 1999;14(2):245–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinatra GM, Brem SK, Evans EM. Changing minds? Implications of conceptual change for teaching and learning about biological evolution. Evol Educ Outreach. 2008;1(2):189–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith MU. Current status of research in teaching and learning evolution: I. philosophical/epistemological issues. Sci Educ. 2010a;19(6–8):523–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith MU. Current status of research in teaching and learning evolution: II. Pedagogical issues. Sci Educ. 2010b;19(6–8):539–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller J. Cognitive load during problem solving: effects on learning. Cogn Sci. 1988;12(2):257–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller J. Implications of cognitive load theory for multimedia learning. In: Mayer R, editor. Camb. Handb. Multimed. Learn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2005. p. 19–30. https://www.cambridge.org/core/books/cambridge-handbook-of-multimedia-learning/implications-of-cognitive-load-theory-for-multimedia-learning/F5F9582CB12C6781FA9C61F6B459D7FC.

  • Tibell LAE, Harms U. Biological principles and threshold concepts for understanding natural selection: implications for developing visualizations as a pedagogic tool. Sci Educ. 2017;26(7–9):953–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tibell LAE, Göransson A, Bohlin G, Schönborn KJ, Höst GE, Jonsson D, et al. Prevalence and characterization of threshold concepts in dynamic visualizations of evolution. Karlstad; 2016.

  • Trend R. An investigation into understanding of geological time among 10- and 11-year-old children. Int J Sci Educ. 1998;20(8):973–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trend R. Conceptions of geological time among primary teacher trainees, with reference to their engagement with geoscience, history, and science. Int J Sci Educ. 2000;22(5):539–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trend RD. Deep time framework: a preliminary study of U.K. primary teachers’ conceptions of geological time and perceptions of geoscience. J Res Sci Teach. 2001;38(2):191–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Truscott JB, Boyle A, Burkill S, Libarkin J, Lonsdale J. The concept of time: can it be fully realised and taught? Planet. 2006;17:21–3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky B, Morrison JB, Betrancourt M. Animation: can it facilitate? Int J Hum Comput Stud. 2002;57(4):247–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vít L, Bláha JD. A study of the user friendliness of temporal legends in animated maps. AUC Geogr. 2012;47(2):53–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood B. Reconstructing human evolution: achievements, challenges, and opportunities. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010;107(Supplement_2):8902–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors’ contributions

JIS had the original idea of the animation. JIS and LAET designed the animation and LAET and JIS designed the study. JIS performed the data collection. JIS and LAET made the annalysis of data JIS original draft of the manuscript. LAET and JIS revised the original manuscript itteratively and made modifications of original manuscript. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all the students who participated in the study, to Henry Fröcklin and Henrik Wahlstedt for technical support and our colleagues in the EvoVis research group. We are also very grateful for valuable opinions on the manuscript from Dr. Gunnar Höst, and to Dr. John Blackwell and Dr. Konrad Schönborn for language reviews.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Availability of data and materials

The animations are available at: http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:liu:diva-153235.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All participants signed a consent form. Since all students were over 18 years old, according to Swedish law, no approval from an ethical committee was required to conduct this study.

Funding

The research was funded by the Swedish Research Council (Vetenskapsrådet) [Grant Number 2012-5344] (EvoVis), The Swedish National School in Science and Technology Education Research (FontD) Swedish Research Council (Grant 729-2013-6871) and Örebro University.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jörgen Ingemar Stenlund.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 6.

Table 6 The questionnaire distributed before the study, to investigate the participants educational background, interests, arithmetic skills, ability to interpret graphical representations of timelines and to compare the size of graphical objects

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stenlund, J.I., Tibell, L.A.E. Visualizing macroevolutionary timescales: students’ comprehension of different temporal representations in an animation. Evo Edu Outreach 12, 8 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12052-019-0099-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12052-019-0099-9

Keywords